For centuries, violinists have sung paeans to the
accomplishments of the Cremona master violinmakers—especially Antonio Stradivari
and Giuseppe Guarneri del Gesu. These two geniuses constructed instruments that
are regarded as the zenith of their art. The violinists of today—at least those
of appropriate means—will pay several million dollars for an original 300
year-old Strad or Guarneri violin. Do these instruments warrant such a price?
For many years, scientists have studied these venerable
violins, trying to figure out what makes their sound so special… or is it special? Is it the varnish they
used? Is it the wood? Does an instrument improve with age, as its body absorbs
the vibrations of its own music? Was there some kind of magic that the masters
had that is a lost art? Various tests have been made, but none so far has shed
any real light on the quandary. The owners of the surviving violins are not
open to someone dissecting their precious treasures, so the years pass, without
an answer. In the meantime, musicians remain convinced that there is something
transcendent about a Cremona violin that escapes today’s artisans.
Finally, last year a French acoustician partnered with an
American violin maker to design an experiment that shed some new light on the
issue. At an international violin competition in Indianapolis, they recruited
21 professional violin players to participate in the experiment. (Previous
listening tests had never used professional players, so they were open to
criticism that lesser talented players were not adequate for the job.) The
researchers were able to talk owners of three precious old violins to allow
their instruments to be used: two were Strads and one was a Guarneri. They
added three new, high-quality violins to the package, so the players had a
total of six instruments to compare.
The tests were conducted in a darkened room and the musicians
wore dark goggles, so they could not see which instrument they were playing.
Two different kinds of tests were conducted: (1) each player was handed two
random violins (one old and one new) and was asked to choose the better one,
and (2) each player was allowed to play every one of the six violins and pick
the one that they’d like to take home.
The results were startling. In the first test, there was no
clear winner of the six violins, but there was one clear loser: one of the
precious Strads. For the second test, only one-third of the 21 professionals
chose an old violin as the best. In fact, two-thirds of them preferred the
sound of a new violin. The musicians were flabbergasted at the results. Before
the test was conducted, they were convinced that they could readily tell the
difference between an old jewel and a new violin. Some of them expressed the
feeling that the test was the experience of a lifetime.
So what’s the message here? It seems to be obvious that some
kind of mystique surrounds the preference for the old masters. Like similar
findings in blind tastings of wine, people can delude themselves about the
superiority of expensive items. When it comes down to an objective test, when
inherent biases are avoided, maybe there is nothing so special about those
high-priced violins. Sure, those old masters were pioneers in making superb
violins, but maybe today’s makers are doing as good a job.
This comparison fascinated me, because it is similar to a
test that I conducted a couple of decades ago, when I taught a course on
musical acoustics at the local university. I loved teaching this course, since
it brought together two of my passions: physics (which I was also teaching at
the time) and music. The basic focus of the course was the study of how musical
instruments make their sounds. The course was a challenge for both the students
and me. Most of them were music majors trying to brave their way through an
intimidating science course (foreign territory to them!) and I was struggling
to make the material comprehensible to non-science types.
My biggest obstacle was to try to convince the music majors
to accept scientific truths about their instruments. These truths had been
arrived at over many years by conducting experiments whose results provide
insight into how Mother Nature works. In science, one accepts the validity of
propositions only after repeatable experiments end with the same results.
Anyone can conduct these experiments—whether neophyte student or experienced
scientist. It’s a democratic process. One is delving into the facts of how the
physical world behaves, and these facts are independent of any human
perception.
The topics that music students study, however, are quite
subjective. Their perceptions and emotions play a central role in how they
discern the quality of a musical performance. It’s in the ear of the beholder.
It’s what one senses or learns to appreciate over time, as one’s ear becomes
educated. There is no scientific instrument, for example, that can tell which
of two flute players is the better performer. Music students learn to accept
the word of their professors as truth, even the gospel. They rely on the
reputation or talent of their teachers. Faith and belief can play a large role
in their discriminating tastes.
More on musical instrument sound quality next time…
No comments:
Post a Comment